Can a Single Cell Recognize Your Face?

By John Horgan
Jun 5, 2005 5:00 AMNov 12, 2019 5:21 AM

Newsletter

Sign up for our email newsletter for the latest science news
 

In the neurosurgery ward of the David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA, Danny, a stocky 21-year-old college student wearing blue pajamas and sporting a wispy goatee, sits on a bed watching one photo after another flash by on a laptop screen. Several macho movie stars appear in rapid succession, including Arnold Schwarzenegger, Steven Seagal, Sylvester Stallone, and Mr. T, the Mohawked brawler who plays Stallone’s rival in the boxing film Rocky III. At first glance, one might guess that Danny has volunteered for a Hollywood survey: Who’s your favorite action hero? In fact, Danny is the real hero. The black cables emerging from the white turban wrapped around his skull hint at his role in investigating a truly profound question: How do thoughts form in the human brain?

Danny suffers from epilepsy, and he has had electrodes temporarily implanted into his brain to monitor seizures. Ideally, the electrodes will pinpoint the neural defect triggering his seizures so that the defect can be surgically removed. During the week or so that the electrodes remain in Danny’s brain, he has volunteered to participate in experiments aimed at understanding the underpinnings of cognition. Such research is quite rare; for obvious ethical reasons, neuroscientists have few opportunities to gather data from deep inside a living human brain.

This particular experiment touches on one of the most challenging puzzles of neuroscience: How do brain cells recognize items as complicated as a toaster oven, the number nine, a zebra, Bill Clinton, or the film character Rocky? Are single cells like transistors in a computer or pixels on a television screen, contributing just minute pieces of information that only when combined with the output of thousands or millions of other cells form the complex pattern that means Rocky? Or can a single neuron learn to recognize that face?


The Clinton cell

New research suggests that an individual brain cell is capable of complex pattern recognition. Electrodes implanted deep in the brains of epileptic patients have detected single neurons dedicated to the recognition of a particular person in different situations and guises. A Clinton cell, for example, would respond not only to various photos of the former president but also to a line drawing.


Most neuroscientists adhere to the pixel view of neurons, arguing that individual cells can’t possibly be clever enough to make sense of a concept as subtle as Rocky; after all, the world’s fastest supercomputers have difficulty performing that pattern-recognition feat. But Itzhak Fried, the neurosurgeon who implanted the electrodes in Danny’s brain and who leads this UCLA research program, believes he has found “thinking cells” in the brains of subjects like Danny. If he’s right, neuroscientists may be forced to overhaul their view of how the human brain works.

A true thinking cell should be able to recognize a person or fictional character even in many different guises. Danny is a big fan of Hollywood action heroes, especially Rocky. He owns DVDs of all five films in the series and never tires of watching them. So, amid the images that pop up on the laptop screen, the research team has included shots that show Rocky running through the streets of Philadelphia, staring longingly at his girlfriend, Adrian, and draped in the American flag after defeating his Soviet rival. Now and then, to test whether a cell’s recognition cuts across sensory modes, Rocky or some other name is spelled out on the laptop screen or uttered by an eerie synthesized voice.

As Danny peers at the laptop, signals stream from more than 60 ultrathin electrodes—each sensitive enough to detect the murmuring of a single cell—and into the cables that emerge from his head. The cables ferry the signals across the room to a cabinet crammed with amplifiers. A computer on top of the cabinet displays the readouts from Danny’s cells as a series of multicolored lines unfolding across a screen. Every now and then, a line jerks upward, as one of Danny’s cells sputters in response to an image or a name. Rodrigo Quian Quiroga, the Argentine neuroscientist overseeing this research session, points to one especially energetic squiggle and whispers, “That’s Rocky.”

The vast majority of modern brain research involves technologies such as magnetic resonance imaging, positron-emission tomography, and electroencephalography. All measure neural activity from outside the skull. Figuring out how brains work with external scanners is like using satellites to study life on a cloud-shrouded planet. Implanted electrodes, by contrast, are like probes that drop down to the planet’s surface. Electrode studies of monkeys and other animals whose brains resemble ours have yielded valuable insights, but these creatures cannot describe their subjective sensations.

A handful of other hospitals are carrying out electrode research that piggybacks on the clinical treatment of patients with epilepsy, Parkinson’s disease, and other neurological disorders. But no research program approaches UCLA’s in experience, sophistication, or published results, says Christof Koch, a neuroscientist at Caltech who has been collaborating with the UCLA group since 1998. “There is no one technique that’s going to give you all the answers” to the riddle of cognition, Koch says. “But this is one that’s very, very good, and we’re getting better at it.”

Fried, the driven yet affable commander in chief of the program, founded it in 1992 after leaving Yale University. Since then more than 100 of his epileptic patients with electrodes implanted in their brains for diagnostic purposes have volunteered as subjects for basic research. This is the first time a reporter has watched Fried’s team at work.

Born and raised in Israel, Fried spends several months a year working at a hospital in Tel Aviv as well as at UCLA. He flew from Israel to Los Angeles on a Sunday, and during a three-hour operation on Monday he drilled a dozen tiny holes in Danny’s skull and inserted the electrodes into his brain. The following day, wearing a white lab coat over aqua scrubs, Fried strode into a conference room packed with researchers who had gathered to discuss plans for Danny. The team included two undergraduates who flew here from the University of Pennsylvania, a few graduate students from UCLA and Caltech, a couple of postdocs, and two physicians.

Fried briskly provided background on the patient: Danny is a bright, friendly young man who is looking forward to working with the research team as a way to “break the boredom” of his hospital stay. “OK, let’s get down to practical issues,” he continued in his distinctive Israeli accent. At rapid-fire speed, he queried the researchers on the status of their “paradigms.” He prefers that term to “experiments,” which might suggest electrodes had been implanted in Danny’s brain primarily for research rather than diagnostic purposes.

The discussion kept returning to problems with data storage and analysis. Several researchers asked for upgrades in equipment for storing data—which the microelectrode experiments generate by the terabyte—and Fried said he’d see what he could do. The researchers also received detailed instructions on how to grapple with a major technical challenge: Electrodes in patients’ brains often detect pulses from two or more nearby neurons at the same time, which may show up in the computer as one big signal. Quiroga has written a program that mathematically unravels overlapping pulses. The process, called cluster cutting, makes it possible to extract more information from the data, at least in principle. But some of Quiroga’s colleagues were still trying to familiarize themselves with the fine points of what the team has dubbed Rodrigo’s code.

The researchers had prepared more than enough studies to keep Danny from becoming bored. One called for him to view computer-generated pictures of celebrities morphing into each other: Mr. T into Will Smith, and Sly into Arnie. The objective: to see if a cell that registers for Sly fires more slowly as the photo gradually morphs into Arnie or just abruptly falls silent. In other words, are face-recognition cells like simple on-off switches, or can they act like dimmers?

Another paradigm, called X-Cab, is designed to yield insights into our episodic memory for places. More than a decade ago microelectrode studies of rats and monkeys revealed place cells that respond when the animals move to a particular spot in a maze. Previous versions of X-Cab, which involves driving a virtual taxi through a cybercity, have confirmed that humans have place cells, too, as well as view cells that respond to specific landmarks and goal cells that respond to the driver’s ultimate destination.

Arne Ekstrom, a UCLA postdoc, and Indre Viskontas, a graduate student, had made preparations for Danny to test-drive a new version of the X-Cab program, which allows the driver to pick up and discharge passengers. Fried asked if they had made the changes he requested in the paradigm. “Almost all of them,” Viskontas replied, adding that she and Ekstrom respectfully disagreed with some of Fried’s requests and wanted to discuss them with him.

Fried nodded. “Any more questions?” he asked, scanning the room. “If not, to work.”

Graphic by Bryan Christie

A supercharged microprocessor

Each of the 100 billion neurons in the human brain is an elaborate processor powered by neurotransmitters. These electrically charged chemicals accumulate at the receptor nodules of antenna-like dendrites and activate the cell body, which in turn shoots an electric signal down the neuron’s output wire, the axon. At the axon’s end, the signal releases bundled packages of additional neurotransmitters, which burst free and power other neurons. Susan Kruglinski

Dendrites

A typical cell in the cerebral cortex, the center of cognition, can receive up to 150,000 contacts from other neurons through its dendrites (upper left). Branches are covered in thousands of tiny receptor nodules called synapses. More than 50 varieties of neurotransmitters may make contact by binding to molecules at the synapse, where they become electric.

Axon

The positively charged cell blasts electricity down the wiry axon (lower left). A bare axon conducts a signal at one to 20 miles per hour, while axons with a fatty insulation called myelin conduct electricity at up to 270 miles per hour, the speed of wind in an F5 tornado.

Action potential

When about 0.1 volt kicks in (1/100,000 the strength of a static shock from a rug), negatively charged potassium rushes out of the cell, and positively charged sodium floods in at 100,000,000 ions per second. The cell charge then flips from negative to positive. A neuron can generate action potentials (upper right) at a rate of 300 per second.

Synaptic transmission

Electricity signals the release of neurotransmitters, which exit the axon in packets of about 5,000 molecules (lower right). The chemicals break free en route to the next dendrite synapse. The type of neurotransmitter and the strength of each connection to the synapse affect the functioning of the recipient neuron.

Back in his office, Fried recalled how he ended up overseeing this unusual program. One of his role models was Wilder Penfield, the Canadian surgeon who carried out pioneering operations on epileptics in the 1930s and 1940s. After removing the skullcap of patients, Penfield electrically tickled different spots of their brains with wires and asked them what they felt. Because the brain lacks pain receptors, the patients needed no anesthesia. They reported such sensations as a tingle in the left forefinger, seeing a blue flash, and hearing a low-pitched hum.

This procedure not only helped guide Penfield’s surgical treatment of each patient but also yielded clues to what different parts of the brain do. “He was really looking at the human mind,” Fried said, “but at the same time helping a human being.” Fried’s method is much more refined than Penfield’s. He typically drills a dozen holes in the patient’s skull and inserts a dozen hollow macroelectrodes, which can detect large-scale electric waves emanating from a seizure.

Protruding from the end of each macroelectrode are as many as 10 flexible microelectrodes that can detect the pulses of individual neurons. The patient’s clinical status dictates the placement of the macroelectrodes. In Danny’s case, tests suggested that his seizures originate in his frontal lobes, so Fried inserted most of the macroelectrodes in that region. He embedded one macroelectrode in Danny’s hippocampus, a region that underpins memory and is often implicated in epileptic seizures.

The patient’s clinical health and comfort, Fried emphasized, take precedence over research objectives. Even the most carefully planned paradigm must be set aside if the patient becomes bored, tired, frustrated, gets a headache, or just wants to be left alone. Fried carefully screens prospective colleagues to ensure that they treat his patients like human beings rather than laboratory animals. “The person who will not do well,” he said, “is an obsessive-compulsive animal physiologist who, if he doesn’t control all the variables, falls apart.” But Fried also said he believes that “there is a responsibility” to take advantage of these rare chances to learn more about the behavior of individual neurons, which he calls the building blocks of cognition.

Following Penfield’s example, Fried occasionally does studies that involve stimulating brain cells with minute electric jolts. In 1998 he and three colleagues discovered that a female patient burst into laughter every time they stimulated a spot at the top of her brain called the supplementary motor area. Her hilarity was not just physiological. The woman felt subjective sensations of “merriment or mirth” and displayed a syndrome known as confabulation: She invented reasons for her hilarity, telling the researchers at one point, “You guys are just so funny . . . standing around.”

But most of Fried’s findings, which he has described in more than a dozen papers in such leading journals as Nature, Neuron, and Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, involve not electrically stimulating neurons but passively listening to their chatter as a patient performs various tasks. In one set of experiments, Fried, Koch, and Gabriel Kreiman, a Caltech grad student, found cells that respond both when a subject looks at an image—of a baseball, say, or a woman’s face—and when he closes his eyes and recalls the image in his mind’s eye. The results provide convincing evidence that human perception and imagination share neural circuitry.

The experiments that have attracted the most attention are those supporting the existence of thinking cells. The possibility of such cells has been debated at least since the 1950s, when researchers found single neurons in the visual cortex of cats and other animals that respond to simple stimuli, such as lines oriented at a certain angle or moving in a specific direction, or light of a particular wavelength. Some theorists wondered whether single neurons might also respond to much more complicated stimuli, such as specific people.

Once known as gnostic cells, after the Greek word for knowledge, they were dubbed grandmother cells in the late 1960s by neuroscientist Jerome Lettvin of MIT. Lettvin originally meant the term as a joke. In one paper, he proposed that mother-smothered neurotics such as Portnoy, the hero of Philip Roth’s novel Portnoy’s Complaint, could be cured of their oedipal disorders by having all the mother cells purged from their brains.

Many neuroscientists were skeptical that a single cell could recognize an inanimate object, let alone a person. Even objects as simple as chairs, trees, or buildings come in an almost infinite variety of forms, and the same object looks different from different perspectives and in different contexts. Then in the early 1970s, experiments on monkeys by Charles Gross of Princeton University turned up cells that respond selectively to hands and faces—not specific faces but faces in general.

No one had conducted tests with humans, however, until the late 1990s, when Fried and his colleagues started reporting how epileptic patients reacted to various images. Some neurons were apparently smart enough to comprehend the highly abstract concept of a nonhuman animal. Their neurons fired when the patient was shown a picture of a tiger, an eagle, an antelope, and a rabbit but not when shown pictures of humans or inanimate objects. Other cells favored images only of food, buildings, or human faces. Some cells responded to all faces, but others were picky, firing for male faces but not female ones, or scowling faces but not smiling ones—or finally, faces of specific individuals.

One of the first neurons of this type was the so-called Bill Clinton cell, which was buried deep in the amygdala of a female patient. The cell responded to three very different images of the former president: a line drawing of Clinton laughing, a formal painting of him, and a photograph of him mingling with other dignitaries. The cell remained mute when the patient viewed images of other people, including male politicians and celebrities. Fried’s group found cells in other volunteers that responded in this same highly selective way to actors, including Jennifer Aniston, Brad Pitt, and Halle Berry.

One reason celebrities have played a prominent role in Fried’s experiments is that their photographs are often easier to come by than images of a patient’s own relatives. But as part of her dissertation project on biographical memory, Indre Viskontas, the UCLA graduate student, has for several years been showing patients photographs of family members. She is reluctant to reveal details about her results, which have not been published yet. But she confirms that she has found neurons that respond to a particular relative: father, mother, brother, sister, grandfather, and yes, grandmother. The experiments have also found cells that light up when a patient sees an image of himself. Call them narcissism cells.

Viskontas is wary of overinterpreting these results or others emerging from the UCLA program. She does not believe, for example, that they support the most extreme version of the grandmother-cell hypothesis, in which cells are exclusively and permanently assigned to one person, place, or thing. The past few decades, she adds, have revealed that brain cells are versatile, or plastic, changing their roles in response to new experiences. The UCLA experiments may not be detecting long-term memory but rather so-called working memory, in which cells are temporarily assigned to the job of representing Grandma, Jennifer Aniston, or Rocky only as a result of the stimulation provided by the experiment.

Koch isn’t so sure. It would make sense, he argues, for our brains to dedicate some cells to people or other things frequently in our thoughts. The larger significance of the UCLA experiments, he says, is that neuroscientists may have to change their view of neurons as simple switches, transistors, or pixels. Each neuron may be more like a sophisticated computer. After all, individual neurons can receive input from more than one hundred thousand other cells, some of which inhibit rather than encourage the neuron’s firing. The neuron may in turn encourage or suppress firing by some of those same cells in complex positive- or negative-feedback loops.

What excites Koch most about the thinking-cell results is the possibility that they may illuminate a fundamental component of cognition. Our comprehension of the world, he says, requires that we ignore much of the data flooding in through our senses. When we turn on a TV or reminisce about a movie, our brains somehow instantly compress raw sensory data into meaningful concepts and categories. This feat may be accomplished at least in part, Koch says, by cells that represent not just this or that particular image of Rocky but “the platonic ideal of Rocky.”

Quiroga notes that a short story by a fellow Argentine, Jorge Luis Borges, spelled out what would happen to us if we lacked this capacity for compression. “Funes, the Memorious” tells the tale of a youth who, after falling from a horse and striking his head, becomes gifted, or cursed, with photographic recall of every minute experience. He is so overwhelmed by the infinitude of his perceptions that he retreats into a darkened room. “To think is to forget a difference, to generalize, to abstract,” Borges writes. “In the overly replete world of Funes there were nothing but details.” Unlike Danny, Funes had lost the capacity to perceive the platonic ideal of Rocky.

In Danny’s hospital room, weighty philosophical issues yield to more practical concerns, like getting a tray on rollers properly positioned over his lap. “I’m not an engineer, just a scientist,” Quiroga says apologetically as he struggles with the balky tray. He eventually succeeds with the help of Emily Ho, who is an engineer and the team’s chief troubleshooter.

As other researchers come and go, Ho remains in Danny’s room, manning the test equipment. When the readouts from Danny’s microwires go haywire, Ho checks lights and other appliances that might cause electrical interference. Within minutes she traces the problem to the remote control that Danny uses to make his bed go up and down. After she unplugs it, the readouts return to normal.

The atmosphere in the room is surprisingly cheery. One reason is the frequent presence of Danny’s father, Bill, owner of a carpeting business. Silence reigns during experiments so that Danny doesn’t get distracted, but between sessions Bill teases both the researchers and his son. At one point, Ho, watching signals from Danny’s neurons scroll across a computer screen, tells him that he’s got “great brain cells.”

“Are you kidding?” Bill exclaims. “He’s got lousy brain cells!”

Danny grins, even more so later after his father fumbles a Styrofoam container of Chinese food, sending chicken chunks skidding across the floor. “Who’s got the lousy cells?” Danny chortles.

Bill turns serious when asked why he and his wife agreed to let their son participate in these studies. “It’s a duty,” Bill says. Danny, Bill points out, has benefited because many other patients before him volunteered to be subjects for research. In the future, people suffering from epilepsy or other brain disorders may benefit from what the UCLA team learns from Danny.

For his part, Danny says he enjoys hanging out with scientists and doing experiments—“as long as there’s no math.


Visionary applications Pioneering studies of thinking cells by Itzhak Fried and Christof Koch could help advance at least two technological endeavors. One is pattern recognition, particularly artificial vision. Countless researchers, generously funded by military agencies, have tried to develop computer-based vision, with limited success. The Department of Defense is interested in software that can spot missile sites or other items of military interest in satellite images. Since 9/11, moreover, the Department of Homeland Security has funded research on face-recognition programs that can scan crowds in airports or other areas for suspected terrorists. But even the most powerful computers running the latest software programs have difficulty recognizing simple objects when the setting, perspective, distance, or lighting changes. Disregarding the problem of disguises, face-recognition programs can be thrown off by factors such as changes in hair and facial hair, different expressions, and aging. Knowing how brains solve such problems could help software designers.

Studies of thinking cells could also aid efforts to build so-called cognitive prostheses, devices to replace or supplement capacities lost through brain damage. At the University of Southern California, for example, biomedical engineer Ted Berger is building an implantable chip that could augment the signal-processing of memory cells. For now, Berger is basing his design on recordings from live rats and slices of rat hippocampus. Fried and Koch’s data could, in principle, help Berger make the leap to clinical trials in humans in years to come. But Fried is doubtful that healthy people will have chips installed in their brains to enhance their cognitive abilities anytime soon: “I think the notion of invading the brain will be too much for the foreseeable future.” J. H.

1 free article left
Want More? Get unlimited access for as low as $1.99/month

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

1 free articleSubscribe
Discover Magazine Logo
Want more?

Keep reading for as low as $1.99!

Subscribe

Already a subscriber?

Register or Log In

More From Discover
Stay Curious
Join
Our List

Sign up for our weekly science updates.

 
Subscribe
To The Magazine

Save up to 40% off the cover price when you subscribe to Discover magazine.

Copyright © 2024 LabX Media Group